Page 90 - e-Kongre Bildiriler Kitabı
P. 90

Notably, among other emotions, the pandemic has heightened disgust reactions throughout the
               world. Through an increased awareness in the risks of contamination and infection, social pressure for
               better hygiene and cleanliness practices, as well as mandated lockdowns to various degrees imposed by
               states and local governments, the global experience of disgust has sharply risen (Colarossi, 2020). One
               significant impact of this increased disgust reaction is the way in which it impacts students’ ability to
               interact with new information and critically evaluate beliefs and material. This article seeks to explore
               the  impact  of  the  increased  disgust  on  critical  thinking  among  students  and  educators  as  well  as
               preliminary considerations which could be utilized to support learning.
                      Disgust  is  often  understood as  a  psychological  response  to a  pathogenic threat  (Fleischman,
               Hamilton, Fessler, & Meston, 2015). It serves as part of a behavioral immune system and is believed to
               have evolved as an adaptive response that helps individuals “avoid activities that would result in the
               acquisition of pathogens” (Lee, Amber, & Sagarin, 2014, p. 1115). In this way the increased disgust
               reaction in response to the pandemic is neither inherently problematic nor unexpected. Conversely,
               disgust can be credited with helping individuals follow health and cleanliness guidelines (Gruijters,
               Tybur, Ruiter, & Massar, 2016). This natural desire to be protected from pathogens and germs helps
               motivate individuals to practice better hygiene and follow expert advice.


                      Broadly speaking, disgust can be understood as a protective mechanism that helps to delineate
               what can be incorporated in the body, becoming part of the self, and what must remain outside and
               separate  or  other.  Disgust  is  thus  a  boundary  enforcer  (Chapman,  &  Anderson,  2012).  It  is  often
               associated with bodily functions and fluids, so that body parts utilized in ingestion and secretion of food
               and waste are considered the most sensitive to intrusion and contamination (Lee, Ambler, & Sagarin,
               2014). Researchers have further noted the liminal focus of disgust that reacts against indistinctness,
               which Sartre noted as experiences that produce confusion between what is self and what is other (Lupton,
               2014; Sartre, 1969). Disgust thereby, exists in a binary, delineates clear boundaries, and has no tolerance
               of the uncertainty of nuance.

                      While a full discussion of the complexities of disgust is beyond the scope of this article, an
               important note is that the domain of disgust that is most clearly evoked by and correlated with the
               COVID-19 pandemic is what is considered contamination-based disgust, as opposed to what researchers
               have identified as core disgust or animal-reminder disgust (Ching, Williams, Siev, & Olatunji, 2018;
               Colarossi,  2020).  The  domain  of  contamination-based  disgust  response  is  connected  to  obsessive-
               compulsive  concerns  and  fear  of  contact  with  out-group  members  or  anyone  categorized  as  other.
               Moreover, contamination-based disgust is often associated with tribal boundaries or group identity and
               differentiation  (Haidt,  2013;  Lupton,  2014).  Fear  of  contamination  through  contact  with  out-group
               members frequently leads to interpersonal boundaries and avoidance behaviors. This is noted even when
               the feared contagion is not an actual transmittable pathogen, but an idea or even sexual attraction (Ching,
               Williams, Siev, & Olatunji, 2018). Contamination-based disgust transcends merely physical, individual
               concerns of contamination to psychological, moral, and cognitive processes and defenses. Disgust is at
               work protecting the self from ideas and thoughts as well as germs and bacteria (Haidt, 2013; Lupton,
               2014).


                      Turning to the realm of education, the impact of this increased disgust response in students and
               educators becomes increasingly concerning. While the focus of higher education encompasses a broad
               range of topics and skills, one common thread in most if not all forms of higher education is a desire to
               increase students’ critical thinking capabilities both as a goal in itself and as a tool to increasing student
               felicity with high-level subject matter (Janssen et al., 2019). However, critical thinking is inherently a
               destabilizing force, focused on questioning and critiquing what someone already believes (Mitchell,
                                                              90
   85   86   87   88   89   90   91   92   93   94   95